The obligatory contour principle (frequently abbreviated OCP) is a hypothesis in autosegmental phonology that states that (certain) consecutive identical features are banned in underlying representations. The OCP is most frequently cited when discussing the tones of tonal languages (stating for example that the same morpheme may not have two underlying high tones), but it has also been applied to other aspects of phonology. The principle is part of the larger notion of horror aequi, that language users generally avoid repetition of identical linguistic structures.
Background considerations
A commonly held conception within phonology is that no morpheme is allowed to contain two consecutive high tones. If two consecutive high tones appear within a single morpheme, then some rule must have applied (Odden 1986). Maybe one of the surface high-tone vowels was underlyingly high-toned, while the other was underlyingly toneless. Then, since all vowels must have tone at the surface (in this hypothetical language), the high tone of the one vowel spreads onto the other. Alternatively, one (or both) of the vowels may have started out low-toned and become high-toned due to the application of some rule; or perhaps there was a low tone between the two high tones that got deleted at some point. Regardless, the OCP claims that there can not have been two consecutive high tones (nor two consecutive low tones, etc.) in the underlying representation of the morpheme, i.e. in the morpheme's lexical entry.
History
The locus classicus of the OCP is Leben (1973), in which it was formulated as a morpheme-structure constraint precluding sequences of identical tones from underlying representations. In autosegmental phonology (Goldsmith 1976), with articulated conceptions about associations between featural melodies and skeletal units (i.e. CV phonology, see McCarthy 1979, McCarthy 1981, Steriade 1982, Clements & Keyser 1983), moraic phonology (Hyman 1985, Hayes 1989), the OCP was considered to be relevant to adjacent singly linked melodies but not to doubly linked melodies. The OCP in this "rules and constraints" era was no longer simply a constraint on underlying forms, but also began to play a role in the course of a phonological derivation. McCarthy (1986) proposed that the OCP can actively block the application of or repair the output of phonological rules, while in Yip (1988), Moira Yip attempted to extend the role of the OCP to trigger the application of rules as well. However, there was also a strong opposition to the OCP as a formal constraint in phonological theory, headed by David Odden. Odden (1986) showed that, contrary to the contemporaneous assumption that constraints were inviolable, an examination of African tonal systems reveals many apparent surface violations of the OCP. A lively debate continued between John McCarthy and Odden for several years, with each adding an extra "anti-" to the title of the previous article of the other - e.g. "Anti anti-gemination and the OCP" (Odden 1988), a reply to (McCarthy 1986).
Debate
In optimality theory (OT) (Prince & Smolensky 2004), the OCP has been again redefined as a violable constraint. Yet many issues as to its precise formal character remain: locality - what is the domain of the OCP (i.e. strict adjacency, etc.) and how is the domain represented in the theory; near-identical sequences - many languages show an OCP-like resistance to sequences of segments that differ in just one distinctive feature; there is a question of this being the effect of the OCP, or possibly some other constraint. If the latter, then there is the question of how this constraint formally relates to the OCP; status as an OT constraint - if the OCP is a single constraint, or the local self-conjunction of markedness constraints (Alderete 1997). These and other issues related to the OCP continue to be debated in phonological theory.
Meeussen's rule
A particular instance of the OCP is Meeussen's rule (Goldsmith 1984), named after the Belgian Bantu specialist A. E. Meeussen, which has been used to explain how a sequence HH tones becomes HL in various Bantu languages.
References
- Alderete, John (1997), "Dissimilation as local conjunction", in K. Kusumoto (ed.), Proceedings of North East Linguistic Society 27 (PDF), Amherst: Graduate Linguistic Student Association, pp. 17–31.
- Clements, George N; Keyser, S. J. (1983), CV Phonology, Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press.
- Goldsmith, John (1976), Autosegmental Phonology. PhD dissertation, MIT.
- Goldsmith, John (1984), Meeussen's Rule. In Aronoff, M. & Oehrle, R (eds.), Language Sound Structure, Cambridge, Massachusetts, MIT.
- Hayes, Bruce (1989), "Compensatory lengthening in moraic phonology", Linguistic Inquiry, 20: 253–306. Scanned 2MB Searchable 5MB
- Hyman, Larry M. (1985), A theory of phonological weight, Dordrecht: Foris
- Leben, William (1973), Suprasegmental Phonology. PhD dissertation, MIT. Distributed by Indiana University Linguistics Club.
- McCarthy, John J (1979), Formal problems in Semitic phonology and morphology (PDF), archived from the original (PDF) on 2006-08-30. PhD dissertation, MIT.
- McCarthy, John J (1981), "A prosodic theory of non-concatenative morphology" (PDF), Linguistic Inquiry, 12: 373–418, archived from the original (PDF) on 2006-09-04.
- McCarthy, John J (1986), "OCP effects: Gemination and antigemination" (PDF), Linguistic Inquiry, 17: 207–263, archived from the original (PDF) on 2006-09-04.
- Odden, David (1986), "On the role of the Obligatory Contour Principle in phonological theory", Language, 62: 353–383, doi:10.2307/414677.
- Odden, David (1988), "Anti anti-gemination and the OCP", Linguistic Inquiry, 19: 451–475.
- Prince, Alan; Smolensky, Paul (2004), Optimality Theory: Constraint Interaction in Generative Grammar (PDF), Malden, MA and Oxford, UK: Blackwell.
- Steriade, Donca (1982), Greek prosodies and the nature of syllabification. PhD dissertation, MIT.
- Yip, Moira (1988), "The Obligatory Contour Principle and phonological rules: a loss of identity", Linguistic Inquiry: 65–100.