Cognitive biology is an emerging science that regards natural cognition as a biological function.[1] It is based on the theoretical assumption that every organism—whether a single cell or multicellular—is continually engaged in systematic acts of cognition coupled with intentional behaviors, i.e., a sensory-motor coupling.[2] That is to say, if an organism can sense stimuli in its environment and respond accordingly, it is cognitive. Any explanation of how natural cognition may manifest in an organism is constrained by the biological conditions in which its genes survive from one generation to the next.[3] And since by Darwinian theory the species of every organism is evolving from a common root, three further elements of cognitive biology are required: (i) the study of cognition in one species of organism is useful, through contrast and comparison, to the study of another species’ cognitive abilities;[4] (ii) it is useful to proceed from organisms with simpler to those with more complex cognitive systems,[5] and (iii) the greater the number and variety of species studied in this regard, the more we understand the nature of cognition.[6]
Overview
While cognitive science endeavors to explain human thought and the conscious mind, the work of cognitive biology is focused on the most fundamental process of cognition for any organism. In the past several decades, biologists have investigated cognition in organisms large[7] and small,[8] both plant[9] and animal.[10] “Mounting evidence suggests that even bacteria grapple with problems long familiar to cognitive scientists, including: integrating information from multiple sensory channels to marshal an effective response to fluctuating conditions; making decisions under conditions of uncertainty; communicating with conspecifics and others (honestly and deceptively); and coordinating collective behaviour to increase the chances of survival.”[11] Without thinking or perceiving as humans would have it, an act of basic cognition is arguably a simple step-by-step process through which an organism senses a stimulus, then finds an appropriate response in its repertoire and enacts the response. However, the biological details of such basic cognition have neither been delineated for a great many species nor sufficiently generalized to stimulate further investigation. This lack of detail is due to the lack of a science dedicated to the task of elucidating the cognitive ability common to all biological organisms. That is to say, a science of cognitive biology has yet to be established.[12] A prolegomena[13] for such science was presented in 2007 and several authors[14] have published their thoughts on the subject since the late 1970s. Yet, as the examples in the next section suggest, there is neither consensus on the theory nor widespread application in practice.
Although the two terms are sometimes used synonymously,[15] cognitive biology should not be confused with the biology of cognition in the sense that it is used by adherents to the Chilean School of Biology of Cognition.[16] Also known as the Santiago School, the biology of cognition is based on the work of Francisco Varela and Humberto Maturana,[17] who crafted the doctrine of autopoiesis. Their work began in 1970 while the first mention of cognitive biology by Brian Goodwin (discussed below) was in 1977 from a different perspective.[18]
History
'Cognitive biology' first appeared in the literature as a paper with that title by Brian C. Goodwin in 1977.[19] There and in several related publications[20] Goodwin explained the advantage of cognitive biology in the context of his work on morphogenesis. He subsequently moved on to other issues of structure, form, and complexity with little further mention of cognitive biology. Without an advocate, Goodwin's concept of cognitive biology has yet to gain widespread acceptance.
Aside from an essay regarding Goodwin's conception by Margaret Boden in 1980, the next appearance of ‘cognitive biology’ as a phrase in the literature came in 1986 from a professor of biochemistry, Ladislav Kováč. His conception, based on natural principles grounded in bioenergetics and molecular biology, is briefly discussed below. Kováč's continued advocacy has had a greater influence in his homeland, Slovakia, than elsewhere partly because several of his most important papers were written and published only in Slovakian.
By the 1990s, breakthroughs in molecular, cell, evolutionary, and developmental biology generated a cornucopia of data-based theory relevant to cognition. Yet aside from the theorists already mentioned, no one was addressing cognitive biology except for Kováč.
Kováč’s cognitive biology
Ladislav Kováč's “Introduction to cognitive biology” (Kováč, 1986a) lists ten ‘Principles of Cognitive Biology.’ A closely related thirty page paper was published the following year: “Overview: Bioenergetics between chemistry, genetics and physics.” (Kováč, 1987). Over the following decades, Kováč elaborated, updated, and expanded these themes in frequent publications, including "Fundamental principles of cognitive biology" (Kováč, 2000), “Life, chemistry, and cognition” (Kováč, 2006a), "Information and Knowledge in Biology: Time for Reappraisal” (Kováč, 2007) and "Bioenergetics: A key to brain and mind" (Kováč, 2008).[21]
Academic usage
University seminar
The concept of cognitive biology is exemplified by this seminar description:[22]
Cognitive science has focused primarily on human cognitive activities. These include perceiving, remembering and learning, evaluating and deciding, planning actions, etc. But humans are not the only organisms that engage in these activities. Indeed, virtually all organisms need to be able to procure information both about their own condition and their environment and regulate their activities in ways appropriate to this information. In some cases species have developed distinctive ways of performing cognitive tasks. But in many cases these mechanisms have been conserved and modified in other species. This course will focus on a variety of organisms not usually considered in cognitive science such as bacteria, planaria, leeches, fruit flies, bees, birds and various rodents, asking about the sorts of cognitive activities these organisms perform, the mechanisms they employ to perform them, and what lessons about cognition more generally we might acquire from studying them.[23]
University workgroup
The University of Adelaide has established a "Cognitive Biology" workgroup using this operating concept:
Cognition is, first and foremost, a natural biological phenomenon — regardless of how the engineering of artificial intelligence proceeds. As such, it makes sense to approach cognition like other biological phenomena. This means first assuming a meaningful degree of continuity among different types of organisms—an assumption borne out more and more by comparative biology, especially genomics—studying simple model systems (e.g., microbes, worms, flies) to understand the basics, then scaling up to more complex examples, such as mammals and primates, including humans.[24]
Members of the group study the biological literature on simple organisms (e.g., nematode) in regard to cognitive process and look for homologues in more complex organisms (e.g., crow) already well studied. This comparative approach is expected to yield simple cognitive concepts common to all organisms. “It is hoped a theoretically well-grounded toolkit of basic cognitive concepts will facilitate the use and discussion of research carried out in different fields to increase understanding of two foundational issues: what cognition is and what cognition does in the biological context.”[25] (Bold letters from original text.)
The group's choice of name, as they explain on a separate webpage, might have been ‘embodied cognition’[26] or ‘biological cognitive science.’[27] But the group chose ‘cognitive biology’ for the sake of (i) emphasis and (ii) method. For the sake of emphasis, (i) “We want to keep the focus on biology because for too long cognition was considered a function that could be almost entirely divorced from its physical instantiation, to the extent that whatever could be said of cognition almost by definition had to be applicable to both organisms and machines.” (ii) The method is to “assume (if only for the sake of enquiry) that cognition is a biological function similar to other biological functions—such as respiration, nutrient circulation, waste elimination, and so on.”[28]
The method supposes that the genesis of cognition is biological, i.e., the method is biogenic. The host of the group's website has said elsewhere[29] that cognitive biology requires a biogenic approach, having identified ten principles of biogenesis in an earlier work.[30] The first four biogenic principles are quoted here to illustrate the depth at which the foundations have been set at the Adelaide school of cognitive biology:
- “Complex cognitive capacities have evolved from simpler forms of cognition. There is a continuous line of meaningful descent.”
- “Cognition directly or indirectly modulates the physico-chemical-electrical processes that constitute an organism .”
- “Cognition enables the establishment of reciprocal causal relations with an environment, leading to exchanges of matter and energy that are essential to the organism’s continued persistence, well-being or replication.”
- “Cognition relates to the (more or less) continuous assessment of system needs relative to prevailing circumstances, the potential for interaction, and whether the current interaction is working or not.”[31]
Other universities
- As another example, the Department für Kognitionsbiologie[32] at the University of Vienna declares in its mission statement a strong commitment “to experimental evaluation of multiple, testable hypotheses” regarding cognition in terms of evolutionary and developmental history as well as adaptive function and mechanism, whether the mechanism is cognitive, neural, and/or hormonal. “The approach is strongly comparative: multiple species are studied, and compared within a rigorous phylogenetic framework, to understand the evolutionary history and adaptive function of cognitive mechanisms (‘cognitive phylogenetics’).”[33] Their website offers a sample of their work: “Social Cognition and the Evolution of Language: Constructing Cognitive Phylogenies.”[34]
- A more restricted example can be found with the Cognitive Biology Group,[35] Institute of Biology, Faculty of Science, Otto-von-Guericke University (OVGU) in Magdeburg, Germany. The group offers courses titled “Neurobiology of Consciousness” and “Cognitive Neurobiology.”[36] Its website lists the papers generated from its lab work, focusing on the neural correlates of perceptual consequences and visual attention. The group's current work is aimed at detailing a dynamic known as ‘multistable perception.’ The phenomenon, described in a sentence: “Certain visual displays are not perceived in a stable way but, from time to time and seemingly spontaneously, their appearance wavers and settles in a distinctly different form.”[37]
- A final example of university commitment to cognitive biology can be found at Comenius University in Bratislava, Slovakia. There in the Faculty of Natural Sciences, the Bratislava Biocenter is presented as a consortium of research teams working in biomedical sciences. Their website lists the Center for Cognitive Biology in the Department of Biochemistry at the top of the page, followed by five lab groups, each at a separate department of bioscience. The webpage for the Center for Cognitive Biology [38] offers a link to "Foundations of Cognitive Biology," a page that simply contains a quotation from a paper authored by Ladislav Kováč, the site's founder. His perspective is briefly discussed below.
Cognitive biology as a category
The words ‘cognitive’ and ‘biology’ are also used together as the name of a category. The category of cognitive biology has no fixed content but, rather, the content varies with the user. If the content can only be recruited from cognitive science, then cognitive biology would seem limited to a selection of items in the main set of sciences included by the interdisciplinary concept—cognitive psychology, artificial intelligence, linguistics, philosophy, neuroscience, and cognitive anthropology.[39] These six separate sciences were allied “to bridge the gap between brain and mind” with an interdisciplinary approach in the mid-1970s.[40] Participating scientists were concerned only with human cognition. As it gained momentum, the growth of cognitive science in subsequent decades seemed to offer a big tent to a variety of researchers.[41] Some, for example, considered evolutionary epistemology a fellow-traveler. Others appropriated the keyword, as for example Donald Griffin in 1978, when he advocated the establishment of cognitive ethology.[42]
Meanwhile, breakthroughs in molecular, cell, evolutionary, and developmental biology generated a cornucopia of data-based theory relevant to cognition. Categorical assignments were problematic. For example, the decision to append cognitive to a body of biological research on neurons, e.g. the cognitive biology of neuroscience, is separate from the decision to put such body of research in a category named cognitive sciences. No less difficult a decision needs be made—between the computational and constructivist[43] approach to cognition, and the concomitant issue of simulated v. embodied[44] cognitive models—before appending biology to a body of cognitive research, e.g. the cognitive science of artificial life.
One solution is to consider cognitive biology only as a subset of cognitive science. For example, a major publisher's website[45] displays links to material in a dozen domains of major scientific endeavor. One of which is described thus: “Cognitive science is the study of how the mind works, addressing cognitive functions such as perception and action, memory and learning, reasoning and problem solving, decision-making and consciousness.” Upon its selection from the display, the Cognitive Science page offers in nearly alphabetical order these topics: Cognitive Biology, Computer Science, Economics, Linguistics, Psychology, Philosophy, and Neuroscience. Linked through that list of topics, upon its selection the Cognitive Biology page offers a selection of reviews and articles with biological content ranging from cognitive ethology[46] through evolutionary epistemology; cognition and art; evo-devo and cognitive science; animal learning; genes and cognition; cognition and animal welfare; etc.
A different application of the cognitive biology category is manifest in the 2009 publication of papers presented at a three-day interdisciplinary workshop on “The New Cognitive Sciences” held at the Konrad Lorenz Institute for Evolution and Cognition Research in 2006. The papers were listed under four headings, each representing a different domain of requisite cognitive ability: (i) space, (ii) qualities and objects, (iii) numbers and probabilities, and (iv) social entities. The workshop papers examined topics ranging from “Animals as Natural Geometers” and “Color Generalization by Birds” through “Evolutionary Biology of Limited Attention” and “A comparative Perspective on the Origin of Numerical Thinking” as well as “Neuroethology of Attention in Primates” and ten more with less colorful titles. “[O]n the last day of the workshop the participants agreed [that] the title ‘Cognitive Biology’ sounded like a potential candidate to capture the merging of the cognitive and the life sciences that the workshop aimed at representing.”[47] Thus the publication of Tommasi, et al. (2009), Cognitive Biology: Evolutionary and Developmental Perspectives on Mind, Brain and Behavior.
A final example of categorical use comes from an author’s introduction to his 2011 publication on the subject, Cognitive Biology: Dealing with Information from Bacteria to Minds. After discussing the differences between the cognitive and biological sciences, as well as the value of one to the other, the author concludes: “Thus, the object of this book should be considered as an attempt at building a new discipline, that of cognitive biology, which endeavors to bridge these two domains.”[48] There follows a detailed methodology illustrated by examples in biology anchored by concepts from cybernetics (e.g., self-regulatory systems) and quantum information theory (regarding probabilistic changes of state) with an invitation "to consider system theory together with information theory as the formal tools that may ground biology and cognition as traditional mathematics grounds physics.”[49]
See also
References & notes
- ↑ p133 in Lyon and Keijzer (2007).
- ↑ Van Duijn, et al. (2006). "Principles of minimal cognition: Casting cognition as sensorimotor coordination."
- ↑ Lyon and Opie (2007). “Prolegomena for a cognitive biology.”
- ↑ See for example Spetch and Friedman (2006), "Comparative cognition of object recognition.".
- ↑ Baluška and Mancuso (2009). Deep evolutionary origins of neurobiology: Turning the essence of ‘neural’ upside-down.
- ↑ Lyon (2013a) and visit the Comparative Cognition Society to enjoy their publication, Comparative Cognition and Behavior Reviews.
- ↑ [1]See for examples Byrne, et al. (2009), “Elephant cognition in primate perspective.”
- ↑ For instance, Ben Jacob, et al. (2006). "Seeking the foundations of cognition in bacteria.”
- ↑ As one example, see Calvo and Keijzer (2009), "Cognition in plants."
- ↑ Comparative Cognition and Behavior Reviews
- ↑ Lyon and Opie (2007), “Prolegomena for a cognitive biology.”
- ↑ There is no mention of cognitive biology, for example, in Frankish and Ramsey (2012), The Cambridge Handbook of Cognitive Science. Nor is cognitive biology mentioned in Margolis, et al.(2012), The Oxford Handbook of Philosophy of Cognitive Science.
- ↑ At the 2007 meeting of the International Society for the History, Philosophy and Social Studies of Biology, a paper was presented with the apt title: “Prolegomena for a cognitive biology.” See Lyon and Opie (2007).
- ↑ See Brian Goodwin and Ladislav Kováč, discussed below.
- ↑ See p135, 136, and 150 in Huber and Wilkinson (2012).
- ↑ "Chilean School of Biology of Cognition - the Web Page of Humberto Maturana". Archived from the original on 2011-11-01. Retrieved 2014-06-06.
- ↑ Maturana (1970), “Biology of Cognition.”
- ↑ Goodwin (1977), "Cognitive biology." A copy of this four page paper is difficult to find. However, an eight page paper—Goodwin (1978), “A cognitive view of biological process,”—is easy to procure and uses his own 1977 paper as a reference. A study of the other references in the ’78 paper suggests a unique perspective without mention of Maturana or Varela.
- ↑ Goodwin (1977) "Cognitive biology."
- ↑ Goodwin 1976a, 1976b, and 1978.
- ↑ Although many of his publications are difficult to find, this URL has them all: <http://www.biocenter.sk/lkpublics.html>.
- ↑ The seminar, titled "Cognitive Biology," was presented by Professor William Bechtel in the autumn of 2013 by the Department of Cognitive Science at the University of California, San Diego. The seminar description was followed by the schedule for ten sessions and citations for the several papers to be discussed in each session. The papers cover cognitive aspects for a broad range of motile organisms, beginning with two regarding the cognitive abilities of bacteria. Over the course of ten sessions, the speakers include six biologists, two doctors of psychology, and two doctors of philosophy. The initial session featured the presentation of a paper titled to evoke discussion, "Bacterial Information Processing: Is It Cognition?" The paper’s author and host of the seminar sessions was William Bechtel, a philosopher of science who has written extensively on the philosophy and history of cognitive science. See for example Bechtel, W., Abrahamsen, A. and Graham, G. (1998), ‘The life of cognitive science’, in ed. W. Bechtel and G. Graham, A Companion to Cognitive Science (Malden, MA and Oxford: Blackwell Publishers Ltd.), pp. 1–104. A more recent example: Abrahamsen, A. and Bechtel, W. (2012). History and core themes. In K. Frankish and W. Ramsey, The Cambridge Handbook of Cognitive Science. Cambridge University Press.
- ↑ In the preceding paragraph, neither mention nor definition of cognitive biology is made. Rather, context is established through the initial three sentences and an excellent description of cognitive biology is presented thereafter. The original seminar description was followed by the schedule for ten sessions and citations for the several papers to be discussed in each session. The papers cover cognitive aspects for a broad range of motile organisms, beginning with two regarding the cognitive abilities of bacteria. Over the course of ten sessions, the speakers include six biologists, two doctors of psychology, and two doctors of philosophy. The initial session features a talk by Professor Bechtel titled to evoke discussion, "Bacterial Information Processing: Is It Cognition?"
- ↑ Lyon, Pamela (2013a). “Foundations for a Cognitive Biology.” Published on the homepage of the Cognitive Biology Project at the University of Adelaide. http://www.hss.adelaide.edu.au/philosophy/cogbio/ Archived 2014-10-18 at the Wayback Machine
- ↑ Lyon, Pamela (2013a). “Foundations for a Cognitive Biology.” Published on the homepage of the Cognitive Biology Project at the University of Adelaide. http://www.hss.adelaide.edu.au/philosophy/cogbio/ Archived 2014-10-18 at the Wayback Machine
- ↑ This option perpetuates a bias established by the original interdisciplinary grouping that formed the cognitive sciences in the 1970s: artificial intelligence, linguistics, neuroscience, philosophy, anthropology, and psychology. Clarification of cognition was slow in coming, even as computers and robots became more efficient. Some argued that a cognitive agent had to be embodied to interface with the world, thus excluding stationary computers. So a new member was added to the cognitive sciences, i.e., the science of embodied cognition. Both robot frogs and biological frogs were valid subjects for experiment.
- ↑ Such a name implies that this is just another cognitive science, one among equals.
- ↑ Lyon (2013b). “Why Cognitive Biology?” http://www.hss.adelaide.edu.au/philosophy/cogbio/why/ Archived 2014-07-14 at the Wayback Machine.
- ↑ See also Lyon and Opie (2007) “Prolegomena for a cognitive biology.”
- ↑ More detail can be found in Lyon (2006), “The biogenic approach to cognition.”.
- ↑ See Lyon (2006), pps 15-20 for the principles quoted.
- ↑ This is the official website for the Department für Kognitionsbiologie: <http://cogbio.univie.ac.at/home Archived 2016-12-14 at the Wayback Machine>
- ↑ Department für Kognitionsbiologie COGBIO: Department of Cognitive Biology, Faculty of Life Sciences, University of Vienna, Austria
- ↑ "Archived copy" (PDF). Archived from the original (PDF) on 2011-12-26. Retrieved 2014-06-06.
{{cite web}}
: CS1 maint: archived copy as title (link) - ↑ "News and Events | Cognitive Biology Group". Archived from the original on 2016-07-25. Retrieved 2014-06-06.
- ↑ Courses from the Cognitive Biology Group, Institute of Biology, Faculty of Science, OVGU (Otto-von-Guericke University) Magdeburg, Germany.
- ↑ Research at the Cognitive Biology Group, Institute of Biology, Faculty of Science, OVGU (Otto-von-Guericke University) Magdeburg, Germany.
- ↑ "Center for Cognitive Biology".
- ↑ Named on page 143 in Miller, George A. "The cognitive revolution: a historical perspective." Trends in cognitive sciences 7.3 (2003): 141-144.
- ↑ Named on page 143 in Miller, George A. "The cognitive revolution: a historical perspective." Trends in cognitive sciences 7.3 (2003): 141-144.
- ↑ See first page of Von Eckardt, Barbara. What is cognitive science?. MIT press, 1995.
- ↑ Griffin, Donald R. "Prospects for a cognitive ethology." Behavioral and Brain Sciences 1.04 (1978): 527-538.
- ↑ Stewart, J. (1996). Cognition = life: Implications for higher-level cognition. Behavioural Processes 35: 311-326.
- ↑ Ziemke, Tom (2003). What's that thing called embodiment. In Proceedings of the 25th Annual meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 1305-1310). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
- ↑ [55] WIRES (Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews) (2013). Wiley Online Library. John Wiley & Sons, Inc. http://wires.wiley.com/WileyCDA/
- ↑ For consideration of cogbio v cogetho, see p23 in Kamil (1998), “On the Proper Definition of Cognitive Ethology”.
- ↑ Tommasi, et al., page x in Preface. See pps 3-11 for a discussion of cognitive science and biology.
- ↑ Author’s italics, p2 in Auletta (2011), Cognitive biology: dealing with information from bacteria to minds.
- ↑ Auletta (2011), p61. See also, for examples, pps 335 & 340.
Bibliography
- Auletta, Gennaro (2011). Cognitive biology: dealing with information from bacteria to minds. Oxford University Press.
- Baluška, František and Stefano Mancuso (2009). Deep evolutionary origins of neurobiology: Turning the essence of ‘neural’ upside-down. Commun Integr Biol. Jan-Feb; 2(1): 60–65.
- Bechtel, William (2013). Seminar description for "Cognitive Biology" in the "Cognitive Science 200" series for "Fall, 2013" at University of California, San Diego, <http://mechanism.ucsd.edu/teaching/f13/cs200/>. See also http://mechanism.ucsd.edu/teaching/f13/cs200/bacterialinformationprocessing.pdf.
- Ben Jacob, Eshel, Yoash Shapira, and Alfred I. Tauber (2006). "Seeking the foundations of cognition in bacteria: From Schrödinger's negative entropy to latent information." Physica A: Statistical Mechanics and its Applications 359: 495-524.
- Bird, Angela (2010). Review - Cognitive Biology: Evolutionary and Developmental Perspectives on Mind, Brain, and Behavior
- by Luca, Tommasi, Mary A. Peterson and Lynn Nadel (Editors, MIT Press, 2009). Metapsychology Online Reviews, Volume 14, Issue 3.
- Boden, Margaret A (2006). Mind as machine: A history of cognitive science. Vol. 1. Oxford University Press.
- Boden, Margaret and Susan K Zaw (1980). “The case for a cognitive biology.” Proceedings of the Aristotelian Society, 54: 25–71.
- Byrne, R. W., Bates, L. A., Moss, C. J. (2009). “Elephant cognition in primate perspective.” Comparative Cognition & Behavior Reviews, 4, 65-79. Retrieved from http://arquivo.pt/wayback/20160520170354/http://comparative-cognition-and-behavior-reviews.org/index.html doi:10.3819/ccbr.2009.40009
- Calvo, Paco, and Fred Keijzer (2009). "Cognition in plants." Plant-environment interactions: Signaling & communication in plants: 247-266.
- Chomsky, N. (1972). Problems of Knowledge and Freedom. London: Fontana.
- Denton, Michael J., Craig J. Marshall, and Michael Legge (2002). "The protein folds as platonic forms: new support for the pre-Darwinian conception of evolution by natural law." Journal of Theoretical Biology 219.3: 325-342.
- Emmeche, Claus. "Life as an abstract phenomenon: Is artificial life possible?" (1992). Pages 466-474 in Francisco J. Varela and Paul Bourgine (eds.):Toward a Practice of Autonomous Systems. Proceedings of the First European Conference on Artificial Life. The MIT Press.
- Frankish, Keith, and William Ramsey, editors (2012). The Cambridge Handbook of Cognitive Science. Cambridge University Press.
- Goodwin, Brian C (1976a). Analytical Physiology of Cells and Developing Organisms. London: Academic Press.
- Goodwin, Brian C (1976b). "On some relationships between embryogenesis and cognition." Theoria to Theory 10: 33-44.
- Goodwin, Brian C (1977). "Cognitive biology." Communication & Cognition. Vol 10(2), 87-91. This paper also appeared the same year in deMey, M, R. Pinxten, M. Poriau & F. Vandamme (Eds.), CC77 International Workshop on the Cognitive Viewpoint, University of Ghent Press, London, pp. 396–400.
- Goodwin, Brian C (1978). “A cognitive view of biological process.” J Soc Biol Structures 1:117-125
- Griffin, Donald R. "Prospects for a cognitive ethology." Behavioral and Brain Sciences 1.04 (1978): 527-538.
- Huber, Ludwig and Anna Wilkinson. "Evolution of cognition: A comparative approach." Chapter 8 in Sensory Perception. Springer Vienna, 2012. 135-152.
- Kamil, Alan C. (1998). "On the proper definition of cognitive ethology." Animal cognition in nature. Academic Press, San Diego 1-28.
- Kováč, Ladislav (1986a). “Úvod do kognitívnej biológie.” (Published in Slovak with an abstract in English, the title translates as an “Introduction to cognitive biology.”) Biol. listy 51: 172- 190. {Since old copies of Biologické listy are hard to find, see Kováč (2004a) for the republished version which is also in Slovak. Google translates it into English well enough, with some help from the reader using the Google translation tool.}
- Kováč, Ladislav (1986b). The future of bioenergetics. EBEC Reports 4: 26 - 27.
- Kováč, Ladislav (1987). “Overview: Bioenergetics between chemistry, genetics and physics.” Curr. Topics Bioenerg. 15: 331- 372.
- Kováč, Ladislav (2000). "Fundamental principles of cognitive biology." Evolution and cognition 6.1: 51-69. Evolution and Cognition was published by the Konrad Lorenz Institute for Evolution and Cognition Research (KLI) from 1995-2004. That journal was succeeded by a journal titled Biological Theory: Integrating Development, Evolution and Cognition. The paper is archived at this URL < http://www.biocenter.sk/lkpublics_files/C-7.pdf >.
- Kováč, Ladislav (2006a). “Life, chemistry, and cognition.” EMBO Reports 7, 562- 566
- Kováč, Ladislav (2006b) “Princípy molekulárnej kognície.” Kognice an umělý život VI : 215-222. Translation: “Principles of Molecular Cognition.” Cognition and Artificial Life VI: pp. 215–222
- Kovác, Ladislav (2007). "Information and Knowledge in Biology: Time for Reappraisal.” Plant Signaling & Behavior 2:2, 65-73
- Kovác, Ladislav (2008). "Bioenergetics: A key to brain and mind." Communicative & integrative biology 1.1: 114-122.
- Lyon, Pamela (2006), ‘The biogenic approach to cognition’, Cognitive Processing 7(1), 11–29.
- Lyon, Pamela (2013a). Foundations for a Cognitive Biology. Published on the homepage of the Cognitive Biology Project at the University of Adelaide. https://web.archive.org/web/20141018181532/http://www.hss.adelaide.edu.au/philosophy/cogbio/
- Lyon, Pamela (2013b). Why Cognitive Biology? Published on an HTML page linked to Lyon (2013a). https://web.archive.org/web/20140714230036/http://www.hss.adelaide.edu.au/philosophy/cogbio/why/
- Lyon, Pamela C and Jonathan P Opie (2007). “Prolegomena for a cognitive biology.” A conference paper presented at the Proceedings of the 2007 Meeting of International Society for the History, Philosophy and Social Studies of Biology, University of Exeter. Abstract at http://hdl.handle.net/2440/46578.
- Lyon, Pamela, and Fred Keijzer (2007). "The human stain." Pages 132-165 in Wallace, Brendan editor. The mind, the body, and the world: psychology after cognitivism?. Imprint Academic, 2007
- Mandler, George (2002). "Origins of the cognitive (r)evolution". Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences 38 (4): 339–353. doi:10.1002/jhbs.10066.PMID 12404267.
- Margolis, Eric, Richard Samuels, and Stephen P. Stich, editors (2012). The Oxford Handbook of Philosophy of Cognitive Science. Oxford University Press.
- Maturana, Humberto R. (1970). “Biology of Cognition.” Biological Computer Laboratory Research Report BCL 9.0. Urbana IL: University of Illinois. Reprinted in: Autopoiesis and Cognition: The Realization of the Living. Dordecht: D. Reidel Publishing Co., 1980, pp. 5–58.
- Miller, George A. "The cognitive revolution: a historical perspective." Trends in cognitive sciences 7.3 (2003): 141-144.
- Prigogine, Ilya (1980). From Being to Becoming. Freeman, San Francisco.
- Shapiro, J.A. (2007). “Bacteria are small but not stupid: cognition, natural genetic engineering and socio-bacteriology.” Stud. Hist. Phil. Biol. & Biomed. Sci., Vol. 38: 807–819.
- Shapiro, J.A. (2011). Evolution: A View from the 21st Century, FT Press Science, NJ, USA.
- Spetch, Marcia L., and Alinda Friedman (2006). "Comparative cognition of object recognition." Comparative Cognition & Behavior Reviews 1: 12-35.
- Spitzer, Jan, and Bert Poolman (2009). "The role of biomacromolecular crowding, ionic strength, and physicochemical gradients in the complexities of life's emergence." Microbiology and Molecular Biology Reviews 73.2: 371-388.
- Stahlberg, Rainer (2006) “Historical Overview on Plant Neurobiology.” Plant Signaling & Behavior 1:1, 6-8.
- Stotz, Karola, and Colin Allen. "From cell-surface receptors to higher learning: A whole world of experience." In Philosophy of behavioral
- biology, pp. 85–123. Springer Netherlands, 2012.
- Tommasi, Luca, Mary A. Peterson, and Lynn Nadel, eds. (2009). Cognitive Biology: Evolutionary and Developmental Perspectives on Mind, Brain and Behavior. The MIT Press.
- Tinbergen, N. (1963). On aims and methods in ethology. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 20, 410-433.
- Van Duijn, Marc, Fred Keijzer, and Daan Franken. "Principles of minimal cognition: Casting cognition as sensorimotor coordination." Adaptive Behavior14.2 (2006): 157-170.
- Von Eckardt, Barbara. What is cognitive science?. MIT press, 1995.
- Wasserman, Edward A. (1993). "Comparative cognition: Beginning the second century of the study of animal intelligence." Psychological Bulletin 113.2: 211
- Webster, Gerry, and Brian C. Goodwin (1982). "The origin of species: a structuralist approach." Journal of Social and Biological Structures 5.1: 15-47.
- Webster, Gerry, and Brian Goodwin (1996). Form and transformation: generative and relational principles in biology. Cambridge University Press.
- Whitehead, Alfred North. (1929). Process and Reality. Cambridge University Press.
- WIRES (Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews) http://wires.wiley.com/WileyCDA/ (2013). Wiley Online Library. John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
- Zentall, Thomas R., Edward A. Wasserman, Olga F. Lazareva, Roger KR Thompson, and Mary Jo Rattermann (2008). "Concept learning in animals." Comp Cogn Behav Rev 3: 13-45.